A Proposed Relationship Between Circumcision and Neural Reorganization

Journal  Journal of Genetic Psychology, Volume 159, Issue 3, Pages 367-378. Tuesday, 1 September 1998.

Ronald S. Immerman, Wade C. Mackey
Department of Psychiatry, External link Case Western Reserve University Bryan, TX

Abstract

Humans are intensely biocultural beings. The linkages and causal feedback loops among their symbolic world, their cultural world, and their physical bodies can be exquisitely complex and subtle. It is suggested in this article that one cultural event--circumcision--exemplifies that subtlety and complexity. It is hypothesized that circumcision reorganizes the male's sensory somato-cortex to raise the threshold of sexual excitability/distraction. This threshold shift thereby allows the young men of a social group (a) to be slightly more tractable in exectuting corporate activities beneficial to the community and (b) to be slightly more restrained sexually and more cooperative in the pair bond. The practice is accepted because the procedure is deeply ingrained in the ritual and symbolic life of the social group and is applicable to all young males. Suggestions are made on how to test this hypothesis empirically.

CIRP logo Note:

The Circumcision Reference Library chooses articles based on merit. The authors of this article argue that 1) the foreskin is sensory tissue, and 2) removal of the foreskin by circumcision causes sensory deprivation to the brain, and 3) that sensory deprivation to the brain produces brain damage/reorganization, and 4) that sexual excitability is reduced, and 5) there is a net benefit to society. The Circumcision Reference Library believes that the first three arguments are meritorious and this article is included in the Circumcision Reference Library on that basis. The Circumcision Reference Library believes that arguments four and five lack merit.

ALTHOUGH THE NATURE-NURTURE PROBLEM has had an uneven history, there is consensus on one element within the overall argument: behavior, including motivation, is mediated through the central nervous system. Hence, changes in the central nervous system are aligned with changes in behavior. The correspondence is, of course, not necessarily a one-to-one relationship. Nevertheless, it is clear that nontrivial reorganization or lesions to the central nervous system affect behavioral outputs. Depending on the locus of the neural shifts, the change can be in sensory perceptions, internal analysis, or motor outputs.

It is also clear that cultural inputs that surround and immerse the individual are analyzed by his or her central nervous system, and culturally appropriate behaviors are typically emitted. Distinct cultures have their own distinct inputs, implemented via cultural traditions, and individuals will typically behave with the appropriate, culturally sanctioned behaviors.

In this article, we offer an attempt to link a cultural ritual - circumcision - with behavioral changes. The act of circumcision itself is deeply enmeshed in a culture's symboling system. The behavioral changes are purposed to be mediated by neural changes.

Circumcision: Cultural Rituals

Circumcision involves the permanent removal of the foreskin of the male - invariably a young male and often an infant. Diverse religions and cultures have included circumcision in their ritual and religious worlds: Judaism, Islam, tribes in sub-Saharan Africa, tribes of Australian aborigines (Murdock, 1957; for a history and geography of circumcision, see Bryk, 1934). Each group has developed an elaborate set of images, metaphors, symbols, and rationales for circumcision. (For Moslem examples, see Pierce, 1964; Bowen & Early, 1993; cf. Abu-Sahlieh, 1994; for sub-Saharan African examples, see Turnbull, 1962; Winter, 1959; for an Australian aboriginal example, see Tonkinson, 1978. For a biocultural overview, see Immerman & Mackey, in press. For a map of the distribution of levels of circumcision in contemporary sub-Saharan Africa, see Caldwell & Caldwell, 1996. For a Freudian interpretation, see Young, 1962. For the history in the United States of the ongoing medical debate on the value of circumcision, see Gollaher, 1994. For the various positions of the current medical debate on the benefits and costs of circumcision, see Mansfield, Hueston, & Rudy, 1995; Roberts, 1996; Spach, Stapleton, & Stamm, 1992; Taddio, Goldbach, Ipp, Stevens, & Koren, 1995; Taddio et al., 1997; Weiss & Weiss, 1994; Wiswell, 1997; cf. Szasz, 1996.)

Circumcision from the Jewish perspective. The written history of Jewish circumcision is both long and rich and illustrates well the ornate cultural context that surrounds the physical procedure itself. The Encyclopaedia Judaica (1972, pp. 568-576) gives the following perspective on circumcision in Judaism. It originated, according to the Biblical account, from a divine behest. From Genesis 17: 11-12 comes the prescription: Every male among you shall be circumcised. And ye shall be circumcised in the flesh of our foreskin, and it shall be a token of a covenant betwixt Me and you. And he that is eight days old shall be circumcised among you, every male throughout your generations. The covenant involved a promise that Abraham's lineage would be multitudinous and that these multitudes would inherit the land of Canaan if Abraham and his descendants would practice circumcision. Abraham circumcised himself, his son Ishmael, all the males of his household, and his slaves. In the following year, Abraham's second son, Isaac, was born and was circumcised at eight days old. Circumcision was considered so important that the rabbis taught that were it not for the blood of the covenant (circumcision), heaven and earth would not exist. The ritual of circumcision was and is viewed as a sign of faith. Any perceived medical overlay of the practice occurred only recently.

Complementing any religious-symbolic function, the biological question remains: What consequences occur by the removal of the human foreskin?

Biological and Cultural Consequences of Circumcision

Biological consequences. The relevant but sparse literature that deals with the human foreskin tends to tacitly refer to it as a mere flap of skin. Anatomically, however, this reference is not accurate. The intact human foreskin is richly innervated and contains holocrine glands (e.g., Tyson's glands). (For reviews of the development of sensory nerve endings in the human penis, see Dail & Evan, 1974; Halata & Munger, 1986; Sommerova, 1976.) In terms of surface area, the human foreskin represents approximately 36% - more than one third - of the intact penis's skin (Ritter, 1992; also see Taylor, Lockwood, & Taylor, 1996). As a facet of the man's reproductive apparatus, the foreskin seems nontrivial.

Sensation is localized within the brain. If circumcision affects the sensory pathways of the brain, then the implication should be strong that subsequent sexual sensations will also be affected. Studies on the triangulation of the foreskin, the central nervous system, and circumcision have yet to be conducted or, at least, are not easily located. Nonetheless, inferential evidence is available.

Lesions and cortical reorganization. Researchers have demonstrated that if an animal's digit is removed or de-afferentated, the somato - sensory area that serves that digit is both structurally and functionally reorganized, sometimes extensively (e.g., Doetsch, Johnston, & Hannan, 1990; Doetsch, Standage, Johnston, & Lin, 1988; Kelahen & Doetsch, 1984; Kelahen, Ray, Carson, Massey, & Doetsch, 1981; Welker & Seidenstein, 1959). The effects of the digit removal are greater if the trauma occurs early in the organism's development (cf. Florence et al., 1996; Garraghty & Kaas, 1991). The reorganization of the cortex is generally not orderly. Cortical cells that serve the areas adjacent to the removed/de-afferentated digit expand their influence into the region of the cortex that would have served the affected digit. The expansion seems stochastic rather than pre-ordered or patterned.

In addition, as was shown in monkeys, when the affected, damaged area or areas adjacent to the damaged area are stimulated, the threshold for activating the relevant cortical cells is often higher (Wall, Huerta, & Kaas, 1992). Subcortical areas (e.g., the thalamus; Florence & Kaas, 1995; Garraghty & Kaas, 1991) that project to the somato- sensory cortex are also reorganized. Given that the thalamus is a primary sensory switchboard for the brain, any reorganization at this subcortical level would be expected to influence both cortical and subcortical functioning.

When nerves that innervated the hands of monkeys were severed, the cortical-sensory areas serving those hands did not recover responsiveness to cutaneous stimuli (Huerta & Wall, 1987; Wall & Kaas, 1986; also see Pons et al., 1991; cf. Jackson & Diamond, 1981). After de-afferenting digits of the squirrel monkey, Garraghty, Hanes, Florence, and Kaas (1994) noted that much of the sensory cortex normally activated by the removed digit remained unresponsive to cutaneous stimulation. Garraghty et al. concluded that there is a limit to the scope of new receptive fields that cortical neurons can acquire. (For a similar discussion on the owl monkey, see Wall; Kaas, Sur, et al., 1986.) Wall and Kaas (1986) and Wall, Huerta, and Kaas (1992) offered corroborating evidence and interpretation.

Florence, Wall, Garraghty, and Kaas (1988) noted that nerve regeneration is often incomplete and disorderly and that the remaining area of the somato-cortex becomes reorganized. Large zones of such silent cortex persist over time. Similar reorganization of the somato-sensory cortex (after amputation of a digit or after severing nerves) has been reported for cats (Kalaska & Pomeranz, 1982; Metzler & Marks, 1979) and rats (Cusick, 1996; Cusick, Wall, Whiting, & Wiley, 1990; Rhoades, Wall, Chiala, Bennett-Clarke, & Killackey, 1993; Wall & Cusick, 1984).

It appears that any sensory deprivation of a system can also lead to cortical changes. For example, Kandel (1991) noted in his work with ocular dominance that

If one eye is closed during a critical period, the columns [of neurons in the visual cortex] devoted to that eye shrink while those devoted to the open eye expand. This modifiability of the ocular dominance columns is restricted to a relatively short period just after birth
p. 1024; emphasis added

(See Jessell, 1991, for an overview of neuronal reactions to injury.) It is also useful to note that the phantom limb phenomenon (Melzack, 1992) has not been reported or found regarding the circumcised foreskin.

In a review of the literature on the plasticity or malleability of sensory maps, Kaas (1991) offered the following summary:

(a) Reorganization of sensory maps in the brain follows changes in neural activity patterns induced by the relative inactivation of feed forward pathways produced by lesions or by the removal of the (i) sensory surfaces, (ii) peripheral nerves, or (iii) more central structures.

(b) Sensory maps in the brain, both early and late in processing hierarchies, are mutable, but such changes are more dramatic in higher stations. The increased level of change in these higher areas, closer to the cortex, may be because of the accumulation of serial changes and/or the greater potential plasticity.

(c) Sensory maps in the somato-sensory visual and auditory systems in the brain are all capable of change.

(d) Sensory maps within the brain have been shown to change in a wide range of mammalian species (e.g., raccoons, monkeys, rats, cats et al.). The clear implication is that adult plasticity is a feature of all mammalian brains. And, as a large reservoir of data demonstrates, the younger the individual is, the greater is the plasticity in the development of the nervous system. See Kandel, Schwartz, & Jessell (1991) for examples.

(e) There is presently little evidence related to the behavioral consequences of the reorganization of maps in the brain. Nevertheless, partial recoveries of lost abilities which follow central damage include the possibilities of alterations and improvements in sensory skills.

Synopsis. Framed a little differently within the context of circumcision, the removal of the foreskin would be expected to reorganize the somato- sensory cortex of the brain of the affected individual. Because sexual stimuli normally travel to the cerebral cortex via the thalamus and then to those subcortical areas that are aligned with emotion - for example, the septum (Rivard, 1982) - any reorganization of the cerebral cortex would be expected to affect the overall sexual behavior of that individual (for supporting evidence, see Laumann, Masi, & Zuckerman, 1997; cf. Masters & Johnson, 1966). The earlier in the male's life the circumcision occurs, the more impact the ablation would be expected to have on his nervous system and, hence, his behavioral tendencies.

Thus, we argue here that removal of an individual's foreskin has consequences in that person's brain. The removal of the foreskin - part of the male's reproductive apparatus - is hypothesized to affect sexual excitability. The most intuitive direction of any change would be to lower excitability or raise the threshold for sexual arousal.

Circumcision and skin texture. A result of circumcision is the keratinization and desensitization of the surface epithelium of the glans penis. In other words, if the moistening, protective covering of the glans penis were removed, then the skin on the surface of the glans penis would dry out and become toughened and callused. Again, the end result is a desensitization of the man's reproductive tissues. Excitability would be lowered, and the threshold for sexual arousal would be raised.

Benefits to the Community From Circumcision

Cultures, not unlike stars and species of flora and fauna, seem to have a life cycle. They are born. They flourish. They die. Large numbers of cultures from antiquity, such as the Aztecs, the Incas, the Maya, Pharaoh's Egypt, Socrates' Greece, Caesar's Rome, the Vikings, and Babylon, are no more. Some North American Indian tribes are gone. Extant South American Indian tribes are currently destined for eradication. To remain viable and competitive, intact cultural units must solve various communal problems: secure food (especially proteins); secure potable water; maintain internal cohesion and minimize internal disruption (especially caused by young, energetic males); and maintain defenses against marauding young, energetic males from competing groups (see Harris 1974a, 1974b, 1977, for examples of problems met and solutions found for various facets by a myriad of cultural groups).

We suggest that circumcision originated as a mechanism to lower, even if only slightly, sexual excitability/sexual distractions of young men. Because historical records will probably never be found and validated (circumcision was practiced in Pharaoh's Egypt and is thereby at least that ancient), the suggestion is not amenable to proof. Nonetheless, the argument is as follows.

The energy and aggressiveness of young men are valued assets in a society. Dangers from the young men of other groups need to be prevented, or, if not preventable, then thwarted. Internal tasks, such as communal hunts, require the young men's coordinated activities. If distractions from these tasks are minimized - not eliminated, just minimized - then the efficiency of the coordinated task is improved.

Sexual interest of young men in young women is a very real event. Sexual distraction and excitability for the individual man can certainly compete for his attention and focus. Circumcision could raise the threshold for sexual excitability and distraction. That is, circumcision would not eliminate the sex drive, it would just raise the threshold. (For the importance of sensory input to the brain from the man's genitalia for his effective sexual behavior, see Bemelmans et al., 1991; Gerstenberg, Nordling, Hald, & Wagner, 1989; Rowland, Leentvaar, Blom, & Slob, 1991; Xinet al., 1996.) That is, sexual behavior (of males) is not simply motor/reflex driven.

Thus, the argument here is that circumcision is low-grade neurological castration. As a consequence of circumcision, the young men would be expected to be a ply more tractable and a ply less distractible. Fertility is not impaired. Aggressiveness is not impaired. The threshold for sexual excitability is simply raised.

CIRP logo Note:

CIRP believes that the authors exhibit a fundamental mis-understanding of human sexuality. It is more likely that the removal of the erogenous tissue of the foreskin does nothing to lessen sexual excitability, but does lessen the ability of the circumcised male to achieve sexual satisfaction and gratification for himself and his mate, and may, consequently, tend to maintain the individual in a sustained heightened state of sexual excitement, perhaps with a heightened aggressive tendency. The effect of circumcision would, therefore, be more likely to be the opposite to that hypothesized by these authors.

Benefits to the Pair Bond From Circumcision

If, as we hypothesized, circumcision results in raising the threshold for sexual excitability, then a putative benefit would accrue to the institution of marriage. Men's sexual impulsiveness would be lessened a ply. Again, men's sex drive is not removed, just abated. Consequently, the men's sexual behaviors, which are traditionally viewed as quicker and more direct, would be shifted in the direction of the women's sexual behaviors, which are traditionally viewed as slower and less direct. In addition, extramarital sexual activity would be similarly expected to be reduced.1 Enhanced sexual compatibility within a marriage generally enhances the durability and coordination within that marriage. Consequently, strengthened marriages would be a positive vector within any society.

The Trade-Offs Between the Man and His Society

The societal calculus and trade-offs are as follows: The individual male is denied some additional or more intense sexual feelings. The community gains some additional compliance, and women gain some additional satisfaction within the pair bond. If all males are so affected within the group, no one male or cohort of males is at a courting or amorous disadvantage. The problem, of course, is how to convince parents that their sons' genitalia need to be mutilated. The next section addresses that legitimate query.

The Juggernaut of Cultural Inertia

The socialization process in humans begins at birth and is pervasive. Early learning - the systemization of the rules and worldview of a culture - is very resistant to re-systemization. That is, a process similar to imprinting is in evidence. (For classical discussion on imprinting, see Hess, 1973; Lorenz, 1958, 1965; Tinbergen, 1951, 1965; for current discussions, see Bolhuis & Horn, 1992; Bornstein, 1989; Bower, 1993; Cook, 1993; Leland, 1994; for specific discussions on human mating strategies, see Freedman & Gorman, 1993; Rossi & Rossi, 1990.) Once a child is enveloped in the cultural matrix, wherein each major institution reinforces every other major institution, the mind-set of the child is crystallized. As the child grows to maturity, cultural givens are not questioned, they simply are and are about as visible as the wind.

The child, grown to adulthood and parenthood, will pass on his learned givens to the next generation. The culture of a child is presented to that child in ways and by means a child can understand. For example, were the young Western child to ask, Why is the sky blue? or Why cannot a brother marry his sister? or Why is dishonesty not the best policy? the most effective answers adults can give are because it is, because he can't, because it isn't...now go outside and play. Complex feedback loops of economic-political-social-psychological forces are well beyond the ken of all children to understand and most adults to convey. However, Rabbi Moses Maimonides (cited in Ritter, 1992), who was also a physician of note, clearly articulated our thesis in the 13th century, when he wrote about the Jewish version of circumcision:

The bodily injury caused to that organ is exactly that which is desired; it does not interrupt any vital function, nor does it destroy the power of generation. Circumcision simply counteracts excessive lust; for there is no doubt that circumcision weakens the power of sexual excitement.

What is understood by young children is the concept of authority (Piaget, 1954, 1977). Children universally obey parents and elders. Once so inculcated, children are then predisposed to obey the supernatural, deceased ancestors, and omnipotent deities. If the unseen, powerful forces mandate circumcision, then circumcision it will be. Again, if all sons by all parents are affected, then the cultural rule will have less difficulty getting started and less difficulty in being maintained. There is no exception toward which parents can maneuver. If someone is a member of our group and is a young male, then ipso facto, that boy will be circumcised.

The Lack of Universality

The legitimate question can be raised, If the procedure is beneficial to the competitiveness of a society, then why is the procedure not universal? Part of the answer must lie in the variability of both humans and human culture. Circumcision is not found in cultures indigenous to northern climates. Arguably, a harsh winter, over time, selects for characteristics that obviate the need for added young male tractability (for theoretical discussions, see Calvin, 1990; Freedman, 1974, 1979). Similarly, cultural rules that systematically inhibit informal interaction between the sexes would preclude the need for an additional factor in tamping down young men's ardor. In short, there could well be alternative strategies that successfully cope with male sexual assertiveness.

A Hypothesis to Be Tested

The argument presented here has a number of assumptions and conclusions, some untested, some validated. The key untested conclusion is that a circumcised man's brain is reorganized and changed, compared with an uncircumcised man's brain. One of Popper's (1959, 1962) contributions to the philosophy of science is the recognition of the need for falsifiability. A good discipline generates falsifiable hypotheses; a poor discipline does not. With current technology, living tissue (e.g., the brain) can be studied in living individuals. The argument put forward by this thesis is that imaging of the brain via positron emission tomography (PET) and/or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) could detect differences in the somato-sensory cortex when it receives stimuli from the genitalia of circumcised men compared with similar cortical areas of uncircumcised men. These differences ought to be structural as well as functional. Furthermore, additional differences may be detected in other areas of the brain that are involved in sexual functioning.

Were such a hypothesis to be unsupported, then Huxley's definition of a tragedy, a beautiful theory killed by an ugly fact, would be fulfilled. On the other hand, were such a hypothesis to be supported, then the Gordian knot that interlinks human symboling, human behavior, and human neurophysiology can be unraveled one notch.

Note

  1. An additional advantage to the community of reduced extramarital sexual relations - i.e., reduced numbers of sexual partners - is the concomitant reduction in sexually transmitted diseases. (See Immerman & Mackey, 1997, for a discussion on the threats to the commonweal's reproductive capabilities resulting from endemic sexually transmitted diseases.)

References

  1. Abu-Sahlieh, S. A. A. (1994). To mutilate in the name of Jehovah or Allah: Legitimization of male and female circumcision. Medicine and Law, 13, 575-622.
  2. Bemelmans, B. L., Meuleman, E. J., Anten, B. W., Doesburg, H. H., Van Kerrebroeck, P. E., & Debmyne, F. M. (1991). Penile sensory disorders in erectile dysfunction: Results of a comprehensive neuro-urophysiological diagnostic evaluation in 123 patients. Journal of Urology, 146, 777-782. [External link Medline]
  3. Bolhuis, J. J., & Horn, G. (1992). Generalization of learned preferences in filial imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 44, 185-187.
  4. Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 179-197.
  5. Bowen, D. L, & Early, E. A. (1993). Everyday life in the Muslim Middle East. Bloom- ington, IN: Indiana University Press.
  6. Bower, B. (1993). Chicks hatch chemical clues to memory. Science News, 143, 244.
  7. Bryk, F. (1934). Circumcision in man and woman: Its history, psychology and ethnology (D. Berger, Trans.). New York: American Ethnological Press.
  8. Caldwell, J. C, & Caldwell, P. (1996). The African AIDS epidemic. Scientific American, 274, 62-69.
  9. Calvin, W. (1990). The ascent of mind. New York: Bantam.
  10. Circumcision. (1972). Encyclopedia Judaica (pp. 568-576). Jerusalem: Encyclopaedia Judaica.
  11. Cook, S. E. (1993). Retention of primary preferences after secondary filial imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 46, 405-407.
  12. Cusick, C. G. (1996). Extensive cortical reorganization following sciatic nerve injury in adult rats versus restricted reorganization after neonatal injury: Implications for spatial and temporal limits on somatosensory plasticity. Progress in Brain Research, 108, 379-390. [External link Medline]
  13. Cusick, C. G., Wall, J. T., Whiting, H. H., Jr., & Wiley, R. G. (1990). Temporal progression of cortical reorganization following nerve injury. Brain Research, 537, 355-358. [External link Medline]
  14. Dail, W. G., & Evan, A. P. (1974). Neural and vascular development in the human phallus: A light and electron microscopic study. Investigative Urology, 11, 427-438.
  15. Doetsch, G. S, Johnston, K. W, & Hannan, C. J., Jr. (1990). Physiological changes in the somatosensory forepaw cerebral cortex of adult raccoons following lesions of a single cortical digit representation. Experimental Neurology, 108, 162-175. [External link Medline]
  16. Doetsch, G. S., Standage, G. P, Johnston, K. W., & Lin, C. S. (1988). Thalamic connections of two functional subdivisions of the somatosensory forepaw cerebral cortex of the raccoon. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 1873-1886. [External link Abstract]
  17. Florence, S. L, Jain, N., Pospichal, M. W., Beck, P. D, Sly, D. L., & Kaas, J. H. (1996). Central reorganization of sensory pathways following peripheral nerve regeneration in fetal monkeys. Nature, 381, 69-71. [External link Abstract]
  18. Florence, S. L, & Kaas, J. H. (1995). Large-scale reorganization at multiple levels of the somatosensory pathway follows therapeutic amputation of the hand in monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 8083-8095. [External link Abstract]
  19. Florence, S. L., Wall, J. T, Garraghty, P. E., & Kaas, J. H. (1988). Distribution of skin afferents in the spinal cord brain stem nerve section and regeneration: Relation to cortical maps. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 14, 475.
  20. Freedman, D. G. (1974). Human infancy: An evolutionary perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  21. Freedman, D. G. (1979). Human sociobiology: A holistic approach. New York: The Free Press.
  22. Freedman, D. G., & Gorman, J. (1993). Attachment and the transmission of culture: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems, 16, 297-329.
  23. Garraghty, P. E, Hanes, D. P, Florence, S. L., & Kaas, J. H. (1994). Pattern of peripheral deafferentation predicts reorganizational limits in adult primate somatosensory cortex. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 11, 109-117. [External link Medline]
  24. Garraghty, P. E, & Kaas, J. H. (1991). Functional reorganization in adult monkey thalamus after peripheral nerve injury. Neuroreport, 2, 747-750. [External link Medline]
  25. Gerstenberg, T. C., Nordling, J. H, Hald, T, & Wagner, G. (1989). Standardized evaluation of erectile dysfunction in 95 consecutive patients. Journal of Urology, 141, 857-862. [External link Medline]
  26. Gollaher, D. L. (1994, Fall). From ritual to science: The medical transformation of circumcision in America. Journal of Social History, 28, 5-36.
  27. Halata, Z., & Munger, B. L. (1986). The neuroanatomical basis for the protopathic sensibility of the human glans penis. Brain Research, 371, 205-230.
  28. Harris, M. (1974a). Cows, pigs, wars, & witches. New York: Random House.
  29. Harris, M. (1974b). Why a perfect knowledge of all the rules one must know to act like a native cannot lead to the knowledge of how natives act. Journal of Anthropological Research, 30, 242-251.
  30. Harris, M. (1977). Cannibals and king: The origins of cultures. New York: Random House.
  31. Hess, E. (1973). Imprinting. New York: D. Van Nostrand.
  32. Huerta, M. F., & Wall, J. T. (1987). The functional territory of the primate median nerve in the hand skin and the cortical map of the hand. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 13, 250.
  33. Immerman, R. S., & Mackey, W. C. (1997). Establishing a link between cultural evolution and sexually transmitted diseases. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 441-459.
  34. Immerman, R. S., & Mackey, W. C. (in press). A biocultural analysis of circumcision: A kinder, gentler tumescence. Social Biology.
  35. Jackson, P. C., & Diamond, J. (1981). Regenerating axons reclaim sensory targets from collateral nerve sprouts. Science, 214, 926-928.
  36. Jessell, T M. (1991). Reactions of neurons to injury. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Principles of neural science (3rd ed., pp. 258-269). New York: Elsevier.
  37. Kaas, J. H. (1991). Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 14, 137-167.
  38. Kalaska, J., & Pomeranz, B. (1982). Chronic peripheral nerve injuries alter the somatotopic organization of the cuneate nucleus in kittens. Brain Research, 236, 35-47. [External link Medline]
  39. Kandel, E. R. (1991). Cellular mechanisms of learning and the biological basis of individuality. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Principles of neural science (3rd ed., pp. 1009-1031). New York: Elsevier.
  40. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (Eds.). (1991). Principles of neural sci- ence (3rd ed.). New York: Elsevier.
  41. Kelahen, A. M., & Doetsch, G. S. (1984). Time dependent changes in the functional organization of somatosensory cerebral cortex following digit amputation in adult raccoons. Somatosensory Research, 2, 49-81.
  42. Kelahen, A. M., Ray, R. H., Carson, L. V., Massey, C. E., & Doetsch, G. S. (1981). Functional reorganization of adult raccoon somatosensory cerebral cortex following neonatal digit amputation. Brain Research, 223, 151-159. [External link Medline]
  43. Laumann, E. O., Masi, C. M., & Zuckerman, E. W. (1997). Circumcision in the United States: Prevalence, prophylactic effects and sexual practice. Journal of the American Medical Association, 277, 1051-1057.
  44. Leland, K. N. (1994). On the evolutionary consequences of sexual imprinting. Evolution, 48, 477-489.
  45. Lorenz, K. (1958). The evolution of behavior. Scientific American, 199, 67-78.
  46. Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  47. Mansfield, C. J., Hueston, W. J., & Rudy, M. (1995). Neonatal circumcision: Associated factors and length of hospital stay. The Journal of Family Practice, 41, 370-376.
  48. Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little, Brown.
  49. Melzack, R. (1992). Phantom limbs. Scientific American, 270, 120-126.
  50. Metzler, J., & Marks, P.S. (1979). Functional changes in cat somatic sensory-motor cortex during short term reversible epidermal blocks. Brain Research, 177, 379-383.
  51. Murdock, G. P. (1957). World ethnographic sample. American Anthropologist, 59, 664-687.
  52. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
  53. Piaget, J. (1977). The development of thought: Equilibration of cognitive structures. New York: Viking.
  54. Pierce, J. E. (1964). Life in a Turkish village. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  55. Pons, T. P., Garraghty, P. E., Ommaya, A. K., Kaas, J. H., Taub, E., & Mishkin, M. (1991). Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science, 252, 1857-1860. [External link Abstract]
  56. Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.
  57. Popper, K. R. (1962). Conjectures and refutations. New York: Basic Books.
  58. Rhoades, R. W., Wall, J. T., Chiala, N. L., Bennett-Clarke, C. A., & Killackey, P. P. (1993). Anatomical and functional changes in the organization of the cuneate nucleus of adult rats after fetal forelimb amputation. Journal of Neuroscience, 13 , 1106-1119. [External link Abstract]
  59. Ritter, T. J. (1992). Say no to circumcision. Aptos, CA: Hourglass.
  60. Rivard, D. J. (1982). Anatomy, physiology, and neurophysiology of male sexual function. In A. H. Bennett (Ed.), Management of male impotence (pp. 1-25). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
  61. Roberts, J. A. (1996). Neonatal circumcision: An end to the controversy? Southern Medical Journal, 89, 167-171.
  62. Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across the life course. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
  63. Rowland, D. L., Leentvaar, E. J., Blom, J. H., & Slob, A. K. (1991). Changes in penile sensitivity following papaverine induced erection in sexually functional and dysfunctional men. Journal of Urology, 146, 1018-1021.
  64. Sommerova, J. (1976). Pre- and postnatal development of sensory nerve endings in penis in man. Scripta Medica, 49, 267-381.
  65. Spach, D. H., Stapleton, A. E., & Stamm, W. E. (1992). Lack of circumcision increases the risk of urinary tract infection in young men. Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 679-681.
  66. Szasz, T. (1996). External link Routine neonatal circumcision: Symbol of the birth of the therapeutic state. The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 21, 137-148.
  67. Taddio, A., Goldbach, M., Ipp, M., Stevens, B., & Koren, G. (1995). Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain responses during vaccination in boys. The Lancet, 345, 291-292.
  68. Taddio, A., Stevens, B., Craig, K., Rastogi, P., Ben-David, S., Shennan, A., Mulligan, P., & Koren, G. (1997). Efficacy and safety of lidocaine-prilocaine cream for pain during circumcision. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 1199-1201.
  69. Taylor, J. R., Lockwood, A. P., & Taylor, A. J. (1996). The prepuce: Specialized mucosa of the penis and its loss to circumcision. British Journal of Urology, 77, 291-295.
  70. Tinbergen N. (1951). The study of instinct. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  71. Tinbergen, N. (1965). Animal behavior. New York: Time-Life Books.
  72. Tonkinson, R. (1978). The Mardudjara Aborigines. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  73. Turnbull, C. M. (1962). The forest people. New York: Touchstone.
  74. Wall, J. T., & Cusick, C. G. (1984). Cutaneous responsiveness in primary somatosensory (S-J) hindpaw cortex before and after partial hindpaw deafferentiation in adult rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1499-1515.
  75. Wall, J. T., Huerta, M. F., & Kaas, J. H. (1992). Changes in the cortical map of the hand following postnatal median nerve injury in monkeys: Modification of somatotopic aggregates. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 3445-3455.
  76. Wall, J. T., & Kaas, H. H. (1986). Long-term cortical consequences of reinnervation errors after nerve regeneration in monkeys. Brain Research, 372, 400-404.
  77. Wall, J. T., Kaas, J. H., Sur, M., Nelson, R. J., Felleman, D. J., & Merzenich, M. M. (1986). Functional reorganization in somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 of adult monkeys after median nerve repair: Possible relationships to sensory recovery in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 218-233.
  78. Weiss, G. N., & Weiss, B. B. (1994). A perspective on controversies over neonatal circumcision. Clinical Pediatrics, 33, 726-730.
  79. Welker, W. I., & Seldenstein, S. (1959). Somatic sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of the raccoon (Procyon lotor). Journal of Comparative Neurology, 111, 469-501.
  80. Winter, E. H. (1959). Beyond the mountains of the moon. Urbana: The University of Illinois Press.
  81. Wiswell, T. E. (1997). Circumcision circumspection. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 1244-1245.
  82. Xin, Z. C., Chung, W. S., Choi, Y. D., Seong, D. H., Choi, Y. J., & Choi, H. K. (1996). Penile sensitivity in patients with primary premature ejaculation. Journal of Urology, 156, 979-981. [External link Medline]
  83. Young, F. W. (1962). The function of male initiation ceremonies: A cross-cultural test of an alternative hypothesis. The American Journal of Sociology, 67, 379-396. [External link Abstract]

Received December 8, 1997

Address correspondence to Wade C. Mackey, Townshire Manor, Apt 6, 410 Lake St., Bryan, TX 77801

Citation:

The Circumcision Information and Resource Pages are a not-for-profit educational resource and library. IntactiWiki hosts this website but is not responsible for the content of this site. CIRP makes documents available without charge, for informational purposes only. The contents of this site are not intended to replace the professional medical or legal advice of a licensed practitioner.

Top  © CIRP.org 1996-2024 | Filetree | Please visit our sponsor and host: External link IntactiWiki.